The 7th Avenue
Project, 8/8/11
Yale evolutionary
psychologist Paul Bloom investigates the nature of human pleasure, from sex and
food to art, music and fantasy. He says that what we like depends on what we
think, and there may be no such thing as purely physical pleasure. His latest
book is How Pleasure Works: The New Science of Why We Like What We Like.
RP: Now, just to tie this back to evolution, we should
probably state that evolution will get involved in these things to the extent
that a heritable behavior—it has to be heritable, something that can be
passed down to oneÕs descendants—either increases reproduction or
decreases in reproduction and survival. But
thereÕs a great deal that doesnÕt fall into that category, itÕs neither
heritable nor does it really impact whether weÕre able to multiply. And
provided that the species has adequate resources and can reproduce, thereÕs a
great deal of, you know, leisure time—time to do all kinds of crazy stuff
that evolution never really impacted. I
mean, IÕm telling a story but it seems like a plausible story.
PB: I think thatÕs right. I think thereÕs an interesting
counter-argument that IÕm lukewarm about, but IÕve heard it from [evolutionary
psychologists], which is that a lot of the pleasures of the imagination are
so time-consuming—they take up so much of our lives—that they rob
us from doing things that on the face of it would be more useful.
RP: [laughing] to quote the Everly Brothers: Òonly
trouble is gee whiz IÕm dreaming my life away.Ó
PB: [laughing] Exactly! So it seems to be a bad adaptive
strategy to dream your life away.... [He
cites porn, novels, and video games as examples.] What IÕm often tempted to do
is to agree with you, which is natural selection has given us all
these desires, but were smart enough to subvert them and turn them to a
imaginary means.
RP: And evolution will not put its finger on the scale as
long as those things donÕt keep us from reproducing, or donÕt impair our
survival as a species.
PB: Yes. And the question which [evolutionary psychologists]
raise, which is a good one, is: To what extent does that hold? I mean, imagine a hunter–gatherer in a lifestyle where
collecting food and dealing with the social hierarchies is absolutely critical,
where it is a sort of Hobbesian world—a very dog-eat-dog world. And this
hunter-gatherer spends a lot of his time lying on his back and thinking about
poetry and having fantasies. You might imagine he wonÕt live as long as the
person next to him collecting food and so on. So if it turned out that these
imaginary pleasures did lead to a reproductive cost we would then have to ask:
So whatÕs the countervailing benefit that they provide that leads them to be
there?
RP: Right!
PB: On the other hand, if they donÕt—for instance,
if they are only recent enough, if this world of imagination that we live in
now is a fairly modern invention, then it may not have been around long enough
for natural selection to get its hands on it.
RP: Well, you know, I know IÕm entering full bore storytelling
mode here and
inserting my own opinions, but I do think that the state of nature as
imagined by people like Hobbes and even by some modern evolutionists is itself
a kind of made up thing that involves constant fighting and struggle and all of
that, but in fact if you look at pre-technological cultures they do have a lot
of leisure time to sit around and tell stories and weave baskets or do cave
paintings or whatever, and so a lot of their time does seem to be consumed with
these seemingly useless aesthetic activities or imaginal activities. I think we
may have always had that going on.
PB: I agree, although where I would focus on is your phrase Òseemingly
useless.Ó
RP: Yeah!
PB: Stephen Pinker and I wrote a long time ago an article
called ÒNatural Language and Natural Selection,Ó where we argued for an
adaptation of story, an account of human language. And what we point out in the course of doing so is that youÕd
think hunter–gatherers are spending all their time grunting and running
around, but what they spend a huge
amount of time doing is talking—they spend a huge amount of time telling
stories.
RP: Right!
PB: And then you might argue that those examples—and
the more general examples of a lot of our imaginary activity—may not
be as useless as they seem. So for
instance, a lot of language and gossip is a wonderful and important information
transfer and has a lot to do with status. Your status as a
hunter–gatherer and your status as a contemporary Westerner right now
rests not so much on how big and strong you are; it rests on how
verbal you are. And how charming a person
you are—how funny you are. How good a storyteller you are; how
imaginative you are. And this pushes
toward the view which I am, again, agnostic about; that there might
be an adaptational benefit to a lot of our imaginative recreations.
RP: Yeah, IÕm a little skeptical in this regard. A lot of these stories, you know, about
all social functions being adaptive
contend that you have to go through this process of negotiating the social
relationships in order to bond, and I would think that if evolution wanted us
to bond it would just cause us to bond. You know, I mean, it would be very
simple to have mechanisms in place that wouldnÕt involve all the ritual, all of
the performances, all of the behaviors that simply caused us to bond as
community, or to affiliate.
PB: I absolutely agree with you on this. The claim that
things have adapted for social bonding or for group cohesion seems to be
circular or almost magical. And the same
with claims that they evolved to boost our self-esteem. ThereÕs all these sort
of weird pseudo-adaptations that [evolutionary psychologist] posits, benefits
that one would never need to have. But thereÕs other benefits that are worth
taking more seriously. Here is one, again which I am attracted to, which is we
spend a huge amount of our time daydreaming; living in imaginary worlds. Sometimes they initiate themselves. Nowadays we get
them from the web, TV, movies, or books. ItÕs possible that this does nothing
for us. But itÕs also possible that this serves actually a fairly
useful adaptive function in that we treat it as a form of play, and play is practice. Physical play is practicing
physical activities, like fighting and running. Imaginative play is
practicing what it would be like to live in different alternative worlds. So a lot of what we do in fantasy is imagine Òwhat
would my life be if it was this; if it were that?Ó And we plan; we work through
certain things. And my hunch—itÕs not more than a hunch—, but my
hunch is that if you were to strip away the power to daydream from a
person he or she would be grossly impaired.
RP: I would think so. Is there—I mean I know youÕre
not Oliver Sachs—a pathology that results from an inability to daydream,
or an impaired ability to daydream?
PB: Well, the closest thing I would give, of course, would
be autism, or other disorders on the autistic spectrum disorder, like
aspergers, which do involve an impairment in pretend, in play, in the
imagination. And such kids have terrible problems making their way through the
world É. So, if youÕre asking is there a case where thereÕs people who just
lose the ability to daydream and imagine and everything else is intact, that
would be the perfect case to test what IÕm talking about, but I donÕt think
such cases exist. I donÕt even think such cases even could exist. I think
the ability for imagination is so intricately tied with other capacities, like social abilities, that itÕs not a module that
you could just pluck out and leave everything intact.
RP: I see. I see. As you point out, a lot of our
pleasure—maybe the majority of our pleasure—has a component of
imagination and fantasy in it, whether itÕs a pleasure we take in works of art,
where we imagine the story behind the work, or fiction, or movies, or TV, or
performances, or playing video games, or daydreaming. A huge part of our lives
is spent in a kind of made up world. But we get real pleasure from it; the
pleasure is as real in some cases as the pleasure we get from some material
circumstances. Right?
PB: Yes, absolutely! If you were to tell people they had to
give up one pleasure, I think some people might give up the pleasure of food,
some people would give up the pleasure of sex, some people would give up sport,
but I think it would be a big mistake to give up the pleasure of
imagination—you would find your life bereft.
RP: Ah, yes indeed! ... But this distinction between the
real and the imaginary—on close inspection thatÕs a hard distinction to
draw. I mean, if IÕm fantasizing about having
superpowers and taking over the earth thatÕs clearly fantasy. But if IÕm
thinking about my finances right now—just, you know, Òoh, if I invest
this way IÕll make this much money, or wouldnÕt it be nice if a year from now I
could buy that house I covet—thatÕs also fantasy! I mean, some people
call that real planning, but thatÕs also fantasy. That may never come to pass;
it might involve all kinds of unrealistic scenarios. You know, if I
think about my success in life, thatÕs a social construct—thatÕs a made-up
thing; thatÕs not a physical reality. I
mean, the majority of what we think about is in some sense made
up—itÕs in our heads.
PB: I think thatÕs
right. I think you could think of pleasure and imagination as two
overlapping circles. There is some imagination that doesnÕt have
much to do with pleasure; itÕs not there for pleasure.... There are some
pleasures that have not much to do with imagination.... LetÕs use the term
imagination for cases where one is aware that theyÕre dealing with something
thatÕs beyond the real. This is a crude
rough and ready definition. So that would include daydreaming and
movies and books, and would include things you spend hours on each day. But it doesnÕt include, you know, scratching
yourself and running around the block and having sex and eating a good meal. So
a lot of pleasures arenÕt really imaginary in the interesting sense. But then
thereÕs a huge overlap. If you think of these as two circles, they
overlap, and you have all of these cases where imagination can give rise to
pleasure....
RP: You know in reading your book or about imaginary
pleasures or imagined activities that give rise to pleasure, and this distinction between the
imaginary the real, which I have serious doubts about—I donÕt think
thereÕs a strict line, I donÕt think
itÕs very easy to say (you know, I guess IÕm one of those idealist types,
philosophically speaking, that think itÕs very hard to call something
absolutely real). Nonetheless, it occurred
to me that what we think of as real and what we think of as imaginary, or
made-up, or artificial really does matter in pleasure, and the interesting
thing is a lot of times the pleasure seems come from being suspended between
the two. When someone writes a memoir they often are hit with this—ÓNow you
really made some of this up didnÕt you; you really made some of this up?Ó And
when someone writes fiction (and I interview fiction authors), you know, people
are always asking them: ÒIsnÕt this really true. IsnÕt this really true?Ó ItÕs
as though people wanted it to be sort of both ways. When we look at a
beautiful landscape, sometimes we say:
ÒGod, itÕs just like a painting—thatÕs really great!Ó When we look at a
painting of a landscape, we say: ÒWow, it looks just like a landscape!Ó We sort
of like being suspended—we like this feeling of being suspended between
the two things. Part of the pleasure of looking at a painting for me is knowing
that itÕs artificial, if it happens to be a naturalistic painting, but also
seeing through it to the thing represented. I wouldnÕt be as interested in it
if it was the actual view, you know. IÕm interested in that strange tension
between the created and the natural and real.
PB: I
think thatÕs right. I think thatÕs very close to the treatment I give of some
movies and books—particularly movies where whatÕs critical for the
appreciation of a movie is knowing itÕs a movie....
____________________________________________________
The Tower of Song Program
8/8/11
ÒThe Imagination of Albion: the Summertime in EnglandÓ
Thus in his prophetic poem, The Four Zoas, he gave the name ÒAlbionÓ to the hitherto nameless
ÒEternal Man,Ó who he saw as the father of all mankind, the great ÒAncestor . .
. of the Atlantic Continent, whose History Preceded that of the Hebrews &
in whose Sleep, or Chaos, Creation began.Ó . . . According to Blake, this sleep
meant the loss of the ÒDivine VisionÓ of the Imagination. Thus, the vocation of
the poet is to wake the sleeping giant, or ÒAlbionÕs sleeping Humanity,Ó who
will re-member his true identity and, along with Òthe sons and daughters of
Albion,Ó achieve Eternity through the Imagination and the mystical union of all
things É.
For Blake, the Imagination is a kind of cosmic memory, and
in a way similar to the Celts É imaginal memory lives in the land itself. For
the ancient ancestors of the British Isles, the land itself holds these
stories, as if the storytellers invoke the spirit of place. In early Ireland
the tribeÕs memory was entrusted to a special class of priests called the
Druids. As guardians of an oral-based culture, they were the living libraries
of its history. When the Druid order was destroyed, others, a class called
ÒbardsÓ or poet-seers kept the flame of the memory tradition alive. (Blake definitely belonged to this
class of poet-seers and bards) . . . . Together with a lesser order of the
bards, they kept their culture alive through the Middle Ages and the
Renaissance eras in Ireland, Whales, and Scotland. After this time the
memory-knowledge was passed on into the modern age by generations of village
storytellers known as shanachies. . . .
The English Imagination has been compared, from as early
as the sixteenth century, with a stream, a river, or a fountain, in the same
manner as English poetry. It has
also been compared to an aeolian harp, as in ColeridgeÕs verse . . . . Thus, it
can be said that the English Imagination is like a circle of light; it has no
beginning and end, moves forwards and backwards, and is therefore endless.
. . . . Its narrative Mercian or Northumberland poet
Cynewulf É has often been called the first story-teller in English and is
closely related to a completely lost oral tradition É which suggests that the
island was once full of sounds and sweet airs. The first story in English may have seemed like a song.
Therefore, given all this about the origins of the
Imagination of Albion, my argument here is that the poetry of England rises
naturally from the land itself, like the elemental melody of a land, a land
of dreams.
At least this is what an old French letter (c. 1178) to Nicholas of St.
Albans felt it to be:
Your island is surrounded by water, and not
unnaturally its inhabitants are affected by the nature of the element in which
they live. Unsubstantial fantasies slide easily into their minds. They think their dreams to be visions,
and their visions to be divine. We
cannot blame them, for such is the nature of their land. I have often noticed that the English
are greater dreamers than the French.
To reiterate, for the ancient ancestors of the British
Isles the Imagination, or imaginal memory, lives in the land itself, which
holds mythic stories, and the storytellers—the bards—invoke the
spirit of place. This means that dreams and dream-visions are interwoven within
the fabric of the English imagination—what IÕm calling the Imagination of
Albion. . . .
The beauty of the English Lake District has provided
inspiration for poets and artists. But the first to put it on the mythopoetical
map was William Wordsworth, who became the internationally famous Lakeland
poet. . . .
Concerning my previous discussion of memory and place in
the English Imagination, I want to emphasize that Wordsworth used nature as a
point of departure to recall memories and visions, and that it was the
Imagination that unified man and nature. The great marriage in WordsworthÕs
poem, ÒThe Recluse,Ó is the union of the poetÕs mind with Nature . . . and
Wordsworth will go on to speak of the marriage between the Mind of Man and the
goodly universe of Nature. The Lake-poet envisions the immediate possibility of
this earthly paradise naturalizing itself in the here and now.
How exquisitely the
individual Mind
(And the progressive powers
perhaps no less
Of the whole species) to
the external World
Is fitted:--and how
exquisitely, too--
Theme this but little heard
of among men--
The external World is
fitted to the Mind . . . .
In his autobiographical epic poem, The Prelude (1798-99), Wordsworth, recalls wandering in a mountain gorge and suddenly beholding the moon rising as a symbol of the Imagination:
Imagination--here the Power
so called
Through sad incompetence of
human speech,
That awful Power rose from
the mind's abyss
Like an unfathered vapour
that enwraps,
At once, some lonely
traveller. I was lost;
Halted without an effort to
break through;
But to my conscious soul I
now can say--
"I recognise thy
glory:"